[1] |
郭春彦, 高传吉, 李兵兵(2013). FN400 效应: 外显记忆测量中的概念启动加工. 心理科学进展, 21(9), 1521-1530. |
[2] |
李广政, 林文毅, 王丽娟(2018). SPT效应的编码机制: 项目特异性与项目关联性信息. 心理科学, 41(2), 292-297. |
[3] |
李广政, 王丽娟(2016). SPT效应的自动突显机制: 来自输出监测的证据. 心理学报, 48(3), 238-247. |
[4] |
刘泽军, 王余娟, 郭春彦(2019). 从整合的角度看联结记忆中的项目再认. 心理科学进展, 27(3), 114-122. |
[5] |
律原, 梁九清, 郭春彦(2015). 项目间语义可整合性对联结再认的影响——来自ERPs研究证据. 心理学报, 47(4), 427-438. |
[6] |
王丽娟, 李广政(2014a). 动作记忆: 记忆研究的新范畴. 心理科学进展, 22(6), 953-958. |
[7] |
王丽娟, 李广政(2014b). 动作记忆SPT效应的理论探讨. 心理科学, 37(4), 998-1001. |
[8] |
赵敏芳(2015). 整合编码对联结再认的影响及其认知神经机制. 博士学位论文, 北京: 中国科学院研究生院. |
[9] |
赵敏芳, 傅小兰, 李开云, Zimmer, H. D. (2020). 动作操作与怪异性对联结记忆的影响. 心理与行为研究, 18(2), 145-152. |
[10] |
郑志伟, 李娟, 肖凤秋(2015). 熟悉性能够支持联结记忆: 一体化编码的作用. 心理科学进展, 23(2), 202-212. |
[11] |
周宗泽, 贺金波, 郭永玉(2013). 记忆生存优势: 心理机制及进化心理学解释. 心理研究, 6(2), 3-8. |
[12] |
Addis, D. R., Giovanello, K. S., Vu, M.-A., & Schacter, D. L. (2014). Age-Related Changes in Prefrontal and Hippocampal Contributions to Relational Encoding. NeuroImage, 84, 19-26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.033 |
[13] |
Aly, M., Knight, R. T., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2010). Faces Are Special but Not Too Special: Spared Face Recognition in Amnesia Is Based on Familiarity. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3941-3948. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.005 |
[14] |
Bäckman, L., & Nilsson, L. G. (1984). Aging Effects in Free Recall: An Exception to the Rule. Human Learning: Journal of Practical Research and Applications, 3, 53-69. |
[15] |
Bader, R., Mecklinger, A., Hoppstädter, M., & Meyer, P. (2010). Recognition Memory for One-Trial-Unitized Word Pairs: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials. NeuroImage, 50, 772-781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.100 |
[16] |
Cabeza, R. (2006). Prefrontal and Medial Temporal Lobe Contributions to Relational Memory in Young and Older Adults. In H. D. Zimmer, A. Mecklinger, & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Handbook of Binding and Memory: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience (pp. 315-338). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198529675.003.0024 |
[17] |
Cohen, R. L. (1981). On the Generality of Some Memory Laws. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 22, 267-281. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.1981.tb00402.x |
[18] |
DeMaster, D., Pathman, T., & Ghetti, S. (2013). Development of Memory for Spatial Context: Hippocampal and Cortical Contributions. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2415-2426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.026 |
[19] |
Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). Imaging Recollection and Familiarity in the Medial Temporal Lobe: A Three-Component Model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 379-386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.001 |
[20] |
Donaldson, D. I., & Rugg, M. D. (1998). Recognition Memory for New Associations: Electrophysiological Evidence for the Role of Recollection. Neuropsychologia, 36, 377-395. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00143-7 |
[21] |
Donaldson, D. I., & Rugg, M. D. (1999). Event-Related Potential Studies of Associative Recognition and Recall: Electrophysiological Evidence for Context Dependent Retrieval Processes. Cognitive Brain Research, 8, 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(98)00051-2 |
[22] |
Engelkamp, J. (1998). Memory for Actions. Hove: Psychology Press. |
[23] |
Engelkamp, J., & Dehn, D. M. (1997). Strategy and Consciousness in Remembering Subject-Performed Actions. Sprache & Kognition, 16, Article ID: 94109. |
[24] |
Engelkamp, J., & Jahn, P. (2003). Lexical, Conceptual and Motor Information in Memory for Action Phrases: A Multi-System Account. Acta Psychologica, 113, 147-165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(03)00030-1 |
[25] |
Engelkamp, J., & Perrig, W. (1986). Differential Effects of Imaginal and Motor Encoding on the Recall of Action Phrases. Archiv für Psychologie, 138, 261-273. |
[26] |
Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. D. (1984). Motor Programme Information as a Separable Memory Unit. Psychological Research, 46, 283-299. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308889 |
[27] |
Engelkamp, J., Seiler, K. H., & Zimmer, H. D. (2004). Memory for Actions: Item and Relational Information in Categorized Lists. Psychological Research, 69, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0160-7 |
[28] |
Engelkamp, J., Seiler, K. H., & Zimmer, H. D. (2005). Differential Relational Encoding of Categorical Information in Memory for Action Events. Memory and Cognition, 33, 371-379. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193055 |
[29] |
Engelkamp, J., Zimmer, H. D., & Biegelmann, U. E. (1993). Bizarreness Effects in Verbal Tasks and Subject-Performed Tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5, 393-415. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541449308520127 |
[30] |
Feyereisen, P. (2009). Enactment Effects and Integration Processes in Younger and Older Adults’ Memory for Actions. Memory, 17, 374-385. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210902731851 |
[31] |
Foley, M. A., & Ratner, H. H. (2001). Structures in Activity Memory. Memory for Action: A Distinct Form of Episodic Memory? In H. D. Zimmer, R. L. Cohen, M. J. Guynn, J. Engelkamp, R. Kormi Nouri, & M. A. Foley (Eds.), Memory for Action: A Distinct Form of Episodic Memory? (pp. 112-135). Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[32] |
Giovanello, K. S., & Schacter, D. L. (2012). Reduced Specificity of Hippocampal and Posterior Ventrolateral Prefrontal Activity during Relational Retrieval in Normal Aging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 159-170. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00113 |
[33] |
Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1989). Unitization and Grouping Mediate Dissociations in Memory for New Associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 930-940. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.15.5.930 |
[34] |
Heil, M., Rolke, B., Engelkamp, J., Rosler, F., Ozcan, M., & Hennighausen, E. (1999). Event-Related Brain Potentials during Recognition of Ordinary and Bizarre Action Phrases Following Verbal and Subject-Performed Encoding Conditions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 261-280. https://doi.org/10.1080/713752313 |
[35] |
Holdstock, J. S., Mayes, A. R., Gong, Q. Y., Roberts, N., & Kapur, N. (2005). Item Recognition Is Less Impaired than Recall and Associative Recognition in a Patient with Selective Hippocampal Damage. Hippocampus, 15, 203-215. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20046 |
[36] |
Hunt, R. R., & Einstein, G. O. (1981). Relational and Item-Specific Information in Memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 497-514. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90138-9 |
[37] |
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A Process Dissociation Framework: Separating Automatic from Intentional Uses of Memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513-541. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F |
[38] |
Kormi-Nouri, R. (1995). The Nature of Memory for Action Events: An Episodic Integration View. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 7, 337-363. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541449508403103 |
[39] |
Kormi-Nouri, R., & Nilsson, L.-G. (1998). The Role of Integration in Recognition Failure and Action Memory. Memory and Cognition, 26, 681-691. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211389 |
[40] |
Kriukova, O., Bridger, E., & Mecklinger, A. (2013). Semantic Relations Differentially Impact Associative Recognition Memory: Electrophysiological Evidence. Brain and Cognition, 83, Article ID: 93103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2013.07.006 |
[41] |
Leynes, P. A., & Bink, M. L. (2002). Did I Do That? An ERP Study of Memory for Performed and Planned Actions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 45, 197-210. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(02)00012-0 |
[42] |
Leynes, P. A., & Kakadia, B. (2013). Variations in Retrieval Monitoring during Action Memory Judgments: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials (ERPs). International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87, 189-199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.01.004 |
[43] |
Leynes, P. A., Crawford, J., & Bink, M. (2005). Interrupted Actions Affect Output Monitoring and Event-Related Potentials (ERPs). Memory, 13, 759-772. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210444000377 |
[44] |
Leynes, P. A., Grey, J. A., & Crawford, J. T. (2006). Event-Related Potential (ERP) Evidence for Sensory-Based Action Memories. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 62, 193-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.04.003 |
[45] |
Li, G., & Wang, L. (2015). The Effects of Encoding Modality and Object Presence on Event-Based Prospective Memory in Seven- to Nine-Year-Old Children. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27, 725-738. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2015.1030407 |
[46] |
Li, R., Zhu, X., Yin, S., Niu, Y., Zheng, Z., Huang, X., Wang, B., & Li, J. (2014). Multimodal Intervention in Older Adults Improves Resting-State Functional Connectivity between the Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Medial Temporal Lobe. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6, Article 39. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00039 |
[47] |
Liesefeld, H. R., Liesefeld, A. M., & Zimmer, H. D. (2016). Recollection Is Delayed under Changed Viewing Conditions: A Graded Effect on the Latency of the Late Posterior Component. Psychophysiology, 53, 1811-1822. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12760 |
[48] |
Masumoto, K., Yamaguchi, M., Sutani, K., Tsuneto, S., Fujita, A., & Tonoike, M. (2006). Reactivation of Physical Motor Information in the Memory of Action Events. Brain Research, 1101, 102-109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.033 |
[49] |
Mayes, A., Montaldi, D., & Migo, E. (2007). Associative Memory and the Medial Temporal Lobes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 126-135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.003 |
[50] |
McDaniel, M., & Bugg, J. (2008). Instability in Memory Phenomena: A Common Puzzle and a Unifying Explanation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 237-255. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.2.237 |
[51] |
Mecklenbräuker, S., Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., & Goergens, N. K. (2011). Interactive Context Integration in Children? Evidence from an Action Memory Study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 747-761. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.12.002 |
[52] |
Mecklinger, A. (2000). Interfacing Mind and Brain: A Neurocognitive Model of Recognition Memory. Psychophysiology, 37, 565-582. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3750565 |
[53] |
Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult Age Differences in Memory Performance: Tests of an Associative Deficit Hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1170-1187. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1170 |
[54] |
Nilsson, L.-G. (2000). Remembering Actions and Words. In F. I. M. Craik, & E. Tulving (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory (pp. 137-148). Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[55] |
Nilsson, L.-G., & Kormi-Nouri, R. (2001). What Is the Meaning of a Memory-System Approach. In H. D. Zimmer, R. L. Cohen, M. J. Guynn, J. Engelkamp, R. Kormi Nouri, & M. A. Foley (Eds.), Memory for Action: A Distinct Form of Episodic Memory? (pp. 136-143). Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[56] |
Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of Action Slips. Psychological Review, 88, 1-15. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.1.1 |
[57] |
Nyberg, L., Petersson, K. M., Nilsson, L.-G., Sandblom, J., Åberg, C., & Ingvar, M. (2001). Reactivation of Motor Brain Areas during Explicit Memory for Actions. NeuroImage, 14, 521-528. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0801 |
[58] |
Old, S. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Differential Effects of Age on Item and Associative Measures of Memory: A Meta-Analysis. Psychology and Aging, 23, 104-118. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.104 |
[59] |
Opitz, B. (2010). Neural Binding Mechanisms in Learning and Memory. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 1036-1046. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.001 |
[60] |
Paller, K. A., Voss, J. L., & Boehm, S. G. (2007). Validating Neural Correlates of Familiarity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 243-250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.002 |
[61] |
Rhodes, S. M., & Donaldson, D. (2008). Electrophysiological Evidence for the Effect of Interactive Imagery on Episodic Memory: Encouraging Familiarity for Non-Unitized Stimuli during Associative Recognition. NeuroImage, 39, 873-884. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.041 |
[62] |
Roediger, H. L., & Zaromb, F. M. (2010). Memory for Actions: How Different? In L. Bäckman, & L. Nyberg (Eds.), Memory, Aging and the Brain—A Festschrift in Honour of Lars-Göran Nilsson (pp. 24-52). New York, NY: Psychology Press. |
[63] |
Rugg, M. D., & Curran, T. (2007). Event-Related Potentials and Recognition Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 251-257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.004 |
[64] |
Senkfor, A. J. (2008). Memory for Pantomimed Actions versus Actions with Real Objects. Cortex, 44, 820-833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.03.003 |
[65] |
Senkfor, A. J., Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (2002). Episodic Action Memory for Real Objects: An ERP Investigation with Perform, Watch, and Imagine Action Encoding Tasks versus a Non-Action Encoding Task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 402-419. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361921 |
[66] |
Senkfor, A. J., Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (2008). Enactment versus Conceptual Encoding: Equivalent Item Memory but Different Source Memory. Cortex, 44, 649-664. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.12.004 |
[67] |
Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., & Mecklenbräuker, S. (2009). Decomposing the Memory Processes Contributing to Enactment Effects by Multinomial Modelling. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 61-83. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440701868668 |
[68] |
Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., Mecklenbräuker, S., & Marie Thompson, E. (2006). Decomposing Retrieval and Integration in Memory for Actions: A Multinomial Modeling Approach. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 557-576. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000764 |
[69] |
Tibon, R., Gronau, N., Scheuplein, A.-L., Mecklinger, A., & Levy, D. A. (2014). Associative Recognition Processes Are Modulated by the Semantic Unitizability of Memoranda. Brain and Cognition, 92, 1931. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2014.09.009 |
[70] |
Troyer, A. K., Murphy, K. J., Anderson, N. D., Craik, F. I. M., Moscovitch, M., Maione, A., & Gao, F. Q. (2012). Associative Recognition in Mild Cognitive Impairment: Relationship to Hippocampal Volume and Apolipoprotein E. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3721-3728. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.018 |
[71] |
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and Consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 26, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0080017 |
[72] |
Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441-517. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2002.2864 |
[73] |
Yonelinas, A., Kroll, N., Dobbins, I., & Soltani, M. (1999). Recognition Memory for Faces: When Familiarity Supports Associative Recognition Judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 654-661. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212975 |
[74] |
Zhao, M.-F., Zimmer, H. D., Fu, X., & Zheng, Z. (2020). Unitization of Internal and External Features Contributes to Associative Recognition for Faces: Evidence from Modulations of the FN400. Brain Research, 1748, Article ID: 147077. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2020.147077 |
[75] |
Zhao, M.-F., Zimmer, H. D., Zhou, X., & Fu, X. (2016). Enactment Supports Unitisation of Action Components and Enhances the Contribution of Familiarity to Associative Recognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28, 932-947. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2016.1229321 |
[76] |
Zheng, Z., Li, J., Xiao, F., Ren, W., & He, R. (2016). Unitization Improves Source Memory in Older Adults: An Event-Related Potential Study. Neuropsychologia, 89, 232-244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.06.025 |
[77] |
Zimmer, H. D. (1986). The Memory Trace of Semantic or Motor Processing. In F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (Eds.), Human Memory and Cognitive Capabilities (pp. 215-223). Amsterdam: North Holland. |
[78] |
Zimmer, H. D. (2001). Why Do Actions Speaker Louder than Words? Action Memory as a Variant of Encoding Manipulations or as a Result of a Specific Memory System? In H. D. Zimmer, R. L. Cohen, M. J. Guynn, J. Engelkamp, R. Kormi Nouri, & M. A. Foley (Eds.), Memory for Action. A Distinct Form of Episodic Memory? (pp. 151-198). New York: Oxford University Press. |
[79] |
Zimmer, H. D., & Cohen, R. L. (2001). Remembering Actions: A Specific Type of Memory? In H. D. Zimmer, R. L. Cohen, M. J. Guynn, J. Engelkamp, R. Kormi Nouri, & M. A. Foley (Eds.), Memory for Action: A Distinct Form of Episodic Memory? (pp. 3-24). New York: Oxford University Press. |
[80] |
Zimmer, H. D., & Ecker, U. K. (2010). Remembering Perceptual Features Unequally Bound in Object and Episodic Tokens: Neural Mechanisms and Their Electrophysiological Correlates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 1066-1079. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.014 |
[81] |
Zimmer, H. D., & Engelkamp, J. (1985). An Attempt to Distinguish between Kinematic and Motor Memory Components. Acta Psychologica, 58, 81-106. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(85)90036-8 |
[82] |
Zimmer, H. D., Helstrup, T., & Engelkamp, J. (2000). Pop-Out into Memory: A Retrieval Mechanism That Is Enhanced with the Recall of Subject-Performed Tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 658-670. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.658 |
[83] |
Zimmer, H. D., Helstrup, T., Nilsson, L.-G., Helstrup, T., & Magnussen, S. (2007). Action-Events in Everyday Life and Their Remembering. In T. Helstrup, & S. Magnussen (Eds.), Everyday Memory (pp. 57-92). London: Psychology Press-Routledge. |