[1] |
范琪, 叶浩生(2014). 具身认知与具身隐喻——认知的具身转向及隐喻认知功能探析. 西北师大学报(社会科学版), (3), 117-122. |
[2] |
任琳, 丁道群, 黎晓丹, 梁敏响(2018). 手势促进语言的理解与学习: 具身认知的视角. 心理研究, (2), 119-123. |
[3] |
苏得权, 钟元, 曾红, 叶浩生(2013). 汉语动作成语语义理解激活脑区及其具身效应: 来自fMRI的证据. 心理学报, 45(11), 1187-1199. |
[4] |
叶浩生(2013). 认知与身体: 理论心理学的视角. 心理学报, 45(4), 481-488. |
[5] |
叶浩生(2015). 心理学与身体: 经典传统与现代取向. 心理学探新, (4), 291-298. |
[6] |
叶浩生(2017). 具身认知——原理与应用. 北京: 商务印书馆. |
[7] |
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual Symbol Systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577-660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002149 |
[8] |
Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding Conceptual Knowledge in Modality-Specific Systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 84-91. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00029-3 |
[9] |
Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2010). Embodied Cognition and Beyond: Acting and Sensing the Body. Neuropsychologia, 48, 763-773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.029 |
[10] |
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C. S., & Suwalsky, J. T. D. (2013). Physically Developed and Exploratory Young Infants Contribute to Their Own Long-Term Academic Achievement. Psychological Science, 24, 1906-1917. |
[11] |
Engel, A. K., Maye, A., Kurthen, M., & Konig, P. (2013). Where’s the Action? The Pragmatic Turn in Cognitive Science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 202-209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.006 |
[12] |
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What Memory Is for. Behavior and Brain Science, 20, 1-55. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X97000010 |
[13] |
Glenberg, A. M., & Arthur, M. (2015). Few Believe the World Is Flat: How Embodiment Is Changing the Scientific Understanding of Cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 165-171. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000056 |
[14] |
Glenberg, A. M., & Gallese, V. (2012). Ac-tion-Based Language: A Theory of Language Acquisition, Comprehension, and Production. Cortex, 48, 905-922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.010 |
[15] |
Hansen, D., Siakaluk, P. D., & Pexman, P. M. (2012). The Influence of Print Exposure on the Body-Object Interaction Effect in Visual Word Recognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 113. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00113 |
[16] |
Hargreaves, I. S., Leonard, G. A., Pexman, P. M., Pittman, D. J., Siakaluk, P. D., & Goodyear, B. G. (2012). The Neural Correlates of the Body-Object Interaction Effect in Semantic Processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 22. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00022 |
[17] |
Heard, A., Madan, C. R., Protzner, A. B., & Pexman, P. M. (2018). Getting a Grip on Sensorimotor Effects in Lexical-Semantic Processing. Behavior Research Methods, 51, 1-13. |
[18] |
Inkster, M., Wellsby, M., Lloyd, E., & Pexman, P. M. (2016). Development of Embodied Word Meanings: Sensorimotor Effects in Children’s Lexical Processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 317. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00317 |
[19] |
James, K. H., & Maouene, J. (2009). Auditory Verb Perception Recruits Motor Systems in the Developing Brain: An fMRI Investigation. Developmental Science, 12, F26-F34. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00919.x |
[20] |
James, K. H., Jones, S. S., Smith, L. B., & Swain, S. N. (2014). Young Children’s Self-Generated Object Views and Object Recognition. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15, 393-401. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.749481 |
[21] |
Kiefer, M., & Trumpp, N. M. (2012). Embodiment Theory and Education: The Foundations of Cognition in Perception and Action. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 1, 15-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2012.07.002 |
[22] |
Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The Representation of Abstract Words: Why Emotion Matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 14-34. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021446 |
[23] |
Laakso, A. (2011). Embodiment and Development in Cognitive Science. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 15, 409-425. |
[24] |
Marley, S. C., Levin, J. R., & Glenberg, A. M. (2010). What Cognitive Benefits Does an Activity-Based Reading Strategy Afford Young Native American Readers? The Journal of Experimental Education, 78, 395-417. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220970903548061 |
[25] |
Piaget, J. (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York: Norton & Co. https://doi.org/10.1037/11494-000 |
[26] |
Pulvermuller, F., & Garagnani, M. (2014). From Sensorimotor Learning to Memory Cells in Prefrontal and Temporal Association Cortex: A Neurocomputational Study of Disembodiment. Cortex, 57, 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.02.015 |
[27] |
Siakaluk, P. D., Pexman, P. M., Aguilera, L., Owen, W. J., & Sears, C. R. (2008). Evidence for the Activation of Sensorimotor Information during Visual Word Recognition: The Body-Object Interaction Effect. Cognition, 106, 433-443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.011 |
[28] |
Smith, L. B. (2005). Action Alters Shape Categories. Cognitive Science, 29, 665-679. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_13 |
[29] |
Smith, L. B. (2013). It’s All Connected: Pathways in Visual Object Recognition and Early Noun Learning. American Psychologist, 68, 618-629. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034185 |
[30] |
Smith, L. B., & Samuelson, L. (2010). Objects in Space and Mind: From Reaching to Words. In K. Mix, L. B. Smith, & M. Gasser (Eds.), Thinking through Space: Spatial Foundations of Language and Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[31] |
Smith, L. B., Maouene, J., & Hidaka, S. (2007). The Body and Children’s Word Learning. In J. M. Plumert, & J. P. Spencer (Eds.), The Emerging Spatial Mind (pp. 168-192). New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195189223.003.0008 |
[32] |
Tare, M., Chiong, C., Ganea, P., & DeLoache, J. (2010). Less Is More: How Manipulative Features Affect Children’s Learning from Picture Books. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 395-400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2010.06.005 |
[33] |
Tillotson, S. M., Siakaluk, P. D., & Pexman, P. M. (2008). Body-Object Interaction Ratings for 1,618 Monosyllabic Nouns. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 1075-1078. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.4.1075 |
[34] |
Wellsby, M., & Pexman, P. M. (2014). The Influence of Bodily Experience on Children’s Language Processing. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6, 425-441. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12092 |
[35] |
Wellsby, M., Siakaluk, P. D., Owen, W. J., & Pexman, P. M. (2011). Embodied Semantic Processing: The Body-Object Interaction Effect in a Non-Manual Task. Language and Cognition, 3, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1515/langcog.2011.001 |
[36] |
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2016). Multiple Sensory-Motor Pathways Lead to Coordinated Visual Attention. Cognitive Science, 41, 5-31. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12366 |